Skip to main content

Edward “Sound Patterns” Sapir (1884-1939), left, and
Leonard “The Phoneme” Bloomfield (1887-1949)
A fun part of my job is to introduce beginning students to the joyous and inspiring world of linguistics. The courses I teach most frequently are Language Variation in American English and a course in the history of the English language. There are no prerequisites for these courses, which is an issue but not one I am going to get into now, except to say that it means most of the students have not taken any linguistics courses before, which further means that we have to spend the first few weeks of the semester on a general introduction to the discipline before we can get into anything really juicy.

The general intro involves, among other things, helping students learn how to talk about language. Like everything else, linguistics has a language of its own. Happily, the students usually have little difficulty in understanding the basic terminology, which most of them pick up quickly and can use comfortably to discuss fairly sophisticated topics in a matter of weeks.

But there is one concept that really messes with their heads, and that is The Phoneme. [1]
And rightly so. [2] Unlike a lot of general linguistic terminology, the idea of the phoneme does not necessarily make sense right away. For some linguists, it does not make sense at all. [3] But even for those who find it a useful concept (which I do, and I will countenance no disrespect of my main men Edward Sapir and Leonard Bloomfield), it takes time to understand The Phoneme in all its mystery. [4]

We start off talking about terminology, and in the process, we use the terms as they learn them to help them think about other terms. So we start with what I call the lex terms, the words about words. Every time they see lex-, I tell them, it’s a good bet we’re talking about something vocabulary-related. There’s lexicon, lexical, lexeme. I point out the –eme in lexeme and mention that when they see –eme on the end of a word, a lot of times it will mean unit of, as in lexeme, a unit of vocabulary, or, you know, a word. [5] So far, so good. Same with morpheme, a unit of meaning, the smallest meaningful unit in language, sort of the meaning-atom of the linguistic world. And we get into different kinds of morphemes, and this is usually OK too. 

But then there’s The Phoneme. That’s when the betrayal happens.

"Yes!" I respond enthusiastically to the students who suggest “unit of sound?” as the definition for phoneme. They have learned well that when they see a phon- word, it is going to have something to do with sound. I used to say that the phon- morpheme (see what I did there?) should be an easy one to remember, and I’d hold an imaginary phone to my ear to demonstrate why. "Get it? Phone? Sound?" But increasingly I am encountering students who never use their phones for anything that actually involves speech sounds. And while I am sympathetic to that – one time my phone malfunctioned to where I could not receive any incoming calls, and it turned out to have been going on for two weeks before I noticed it (and I only realized it because it was pointed out to me by irritated friends whose calls I kept missing and not returning because I didn't know about them) – it is not a helpful development for the teaching of linguistic-terminology mnemonics.

But I digress. Which is really what this diary is for. So deal.

Anyway, yes, a phoneme is a unit of sound, specifically a unit of speech-sound, but.... And here’s where the story starts to fall apart.

As everyone who teaches knows, it is a sad thing to see the faces of the students start to light up (“Hey, this really isn’t that bad!”) only to have to stand by and watch as that light immediately begins to fade as they start to realize that you have pulled a fast one on them and are going to switch it up just as they are starting to get the hang of it.

Yes, I say, a phoneme is a unit of speech-sound, but there’s just a little more to it than that. I say I will write down this ever so slightly more complex definition so that they can copy it down and everyone will have it in front of them and then I will explain it thoroughly. I mean the thoroughly part to be reassuring but instantly realize that this is a mistake. It just disheartens them further. (“This really requires ‘thorough’ explanation? Yikes.”)

I write on a clean sheet of paper placed on the doc cam, using a fresh new Sharpie in an extra special awesome color so that they will see that I am still on their side, but most of them are not convinced. They sit, grimly silent, as the definition appears in letters now twelve times the size of God (as the writer Hunter S. Thompson once described a similarly horrific scene [6]) on the large screen in the front of the room:

phoneme = a class of speech sounds that a native speaker will identify as the same sound.
When I finish writing, there is an air of anticlimax and polite expectancy hanging over us throughout the room, but I also sense that it has a slight edge of annoyance to it as well. But I've been in front of much tougher audiences than this one.

So here I go.

"You know how different people pronounce words in different ways?"

A few nod warily. Others -- especially in Language Variation -- look slightly irritated, since that is one of the exact things they are taking the class to learn more about. Several others look discreetly but frantically at their watches. I am among this last group.

"Well, one thing that is part of what it means to 'know' a language is that you know which pronunciation differences you have to pay attention to and which ones you’re supposed to ignore. So like if I say the word cat, and another native speaker of American English also says it but pronounces the vowel slightly differently from how I pronounce it, we can all still understand that we are both talking about the same kind of animal. More or less."

Everyone laughs at the slightly-lowered, slightly-backed [æ] I have in my pronunciation of cat despite how awesomely normal it sounds compared to the crazy raised and tensed and diphthongized pronunciations of [æ] that many native Michiganders seems to favor. Oh, of course I kid, I kid. I mean about the "normal" and "crazy" parts. I'm dead serious about the raising and tensing and diphthongization of [æ].

At this point, I introduce the term minimal pair.

“Does this mean we're done with phoneme?” someone asks.

“Aren’t you sweet,” I respond. I write a new definition:

minimal pair = two words that are phonemically identical except for a single sound in the same position in both words.
“How is that even remotely helpful?” At this point, the voices in the students’ heads are so loud that even I can hear them. Still, I press on and write down some examples of minimal pairs:
sit / seat

lock / rock

pat / bat

"What’s the difference between sit and seat," I ask, "in terms of sound?"

"The vowels," they answer.

"Yes," I reply, "so sit and seat are phonemically identical" (insert meaningful eyebrow raise). "They sound exactly the same – except for the vowel in the middle of the word, which is a single sound in the same position in both words. All the other sounds in both words, meaning the s and the t, are exactly the same, in the same order.”

"Ohhhh," a few murmur.

"Do sit and seat have different meanings?" I ask.

"Well, their meanings are related," they reply.

"Yes, but are they synonymous?" I demand.

"No. Sit is a verb; seat is a noun," they say. (Reason #892 that I love English majors: They are all down with the parts of speech, they can always be counted on to know what synonymous means, and they don't care who knows it.)

"OK, good, yes!"

I write down [Ι] and [i], the phonetic symbols that represent the vowel sounds in sit and seat, respectively. [7]

"So, if changing the vowel sound changes the meaning, the two vowel sounds in sit" -- I point to [Ι] -- "and seat" -- I point to [i] -- "are therefore phonemically distinct from each other. Another way to say this is to say that for [Ι] and [i], each one is a member of a class of speech sounds that a native speaker will identify as the same sound but that they are not members of the same class."

There is some murmuring, but it does not sound overtly mutinous, so I go on.

"And we know [Ι] and [i] are not members of the same class of speech sounds because native speakers will not identify them as the same sound. We know that because all y'all native speakers just said that sit and seat are two different words with different meanings. That means you identified these two vowel sounds as members of two different classes. You just proved that they are. And that means that the difference between [Ι] and [i] is phonemic."

"Ahhh," a few more murmur. "Oh crap," a few others mutter. The ratio is satisfactory.

"So, can we say that [Ι] is a phoneme and [i] is another phoneme?" asks a student.

"Yes," I respond. "That’s it exactly!" I pause in a way I think probably appears to be thoughtful. "At least in English."

I see a horrified flash of "Oh, no, what now?" ripple across many faces.

Because I can’t leave well enough alone, and they know it. I have to go and “ruin everything, always!” (according to a student comment in a course evaluation a few years ago, and I quote).

"You know, I should probably mention that just because [Ι] and [i] are phonemic in English doesn’t mean they are in every language," I say gently.

They wait, withholding judgment. But I know it won't last. I take a deep breath.

"Any of you studying Spanish?"

Some hands go up.

"Yes? Thank God. Well, in Spanish, for example, [Ι] and [i] are not phonemically distinct. That means that the difference between [Ι] and [i] is a difference you have to pay attention to in English, but it’s a difference you can ignore in Spanish. In Spanish, it's just an innocuous little pronunciation variant that no one pays any attention to. So you can imagine how obnoxious it must be for native speakers of Spanish to deal with the distinction between words like sit and seat when they’re learning English because, you know, [Ι] and [i] are not phonemically distinct in Spanish. They've spent their whole lives ignoring the distinction between those two sounds and then all of a sudden they have to start actually noticing it. The sentence 'Sit in the seat' must be particularly infuriating."

At this point, I ask how everyone is feeling about everything we’ve gone over so far. They mostly nod encouragingly. WMU students tend to be very stoic and determined people. They also possess what I believe is a higher-than-average level of empathy.

I casually* ask whether they have ever noticed that native speakers of Japanese who are learning English sometimes seem to mix up [l] and [r] in English words. Yes, many of them have noticed this. Well, I point out, native speakers of Japanese are used to ignoring the differences between [l] and [r] in their native language, but in English, it’s a difference that speakers have to pay attention to. I write down lock and rock. 

(*The students are not fooled.)

"Lock and rock are two completely different words in English" I observe. "Why?"

"Because in English [l] and [r] are members of different classes of speech sounds?" offers a student.

"Yes! That is it exactly. Anyone else have another way to say that?"

"The difference between [l] and [r] is phonemic in English," like twelve students say, most of them sighing and rolling their eyes but also a little bit pleased with themselves, you can tell.

Huzzah! My heart sings. Who doesn't love a happy ending?

Except it is not the end. We will continue to have variations on this conversation for the Next. Three. Months. We will have those conversations because we have to. What a phoneme is will be forgotten. It will be relearned and forgotten again. Quizzes will be failed. Tears will be shed, occasionally even by a student. But it does get easier after this. It is slow going at times, but it does get easier. For all of us.

Notes:

[1] I capitalize The Phoneme here in homage to the great linguist Leonard Bloomfield (1887-1949), whose 1933 book Language includes a chapter so titled. Bloomfield was instrumental to the development of structural linguistics in the United States, and Language is still a fun read, although significant parts of it have since been superseded. This link goes to "The Phoneme" on GoogleBooks, where you can see a preview of Language, which is still in copyright and therefore not available to the public in full text online. Along with Edward Sapir, whose work is also discussed in this diary, Leonard Bloomfield was one of the most important figures in American linguistics in the first half of the 20th century. The full text of his Introduction to the Study of Language (1914) at GoogleBooks is linked here.


[2] The idea of the phoneme as the structuralist linguists conceptualized it is complicated, which is why it took a brainiac like the linguistic anthropologist Edward Sapir (1884-1939) to articulate the relationality of speech sounds. He did this in a 1925 paper, "Sound Patterns in Language," linked here, which is included in Volume 1 of The Collected Works of Edward Sapir (14 volumes), full text available via the Internet Archive. Volume 1: General Linguistics contains an astonishing amount of linguistic brilliance in addition to "Sound Patterns." Enjoy this fantastic (and FREE) resource!


[3] See, for example, "Chomsky's Revolution in Linguistics," by John R. Searle (1972) (yes, that John Searle, author of the landmark 1969 book Speech Acts: An Essay in the Philosophy of Language and a ton of other important books and articles).


[4] See this interesting and accessible (although somewhat oversimplified) overview of modern linguistics by C. John Holcombe on the TextEtc site. It begins with Saussure's introduction of what became structuralism and follows its development in the U.S.


[5] There's a smidge more to it than that, but unit of vocabulary or, you know, word, is really all we need with it in these introductory-level classes. There is more here if you want it.


[6] The narrator in Hunter S. Thompson's Fear and Loathing in Las Vegas (1971) notes that "any freak with $1.98 can walk into the Circus-Circus and suddenly appear in the sky over downtown Las Vegas twelve times the size of God, howling anything that comes into his head."


[7] The Linguistics Program at the University of Victoria has some great resources, including the chart linked here of the alphabet of the International Phonetic Association (IPA). It includes audio files so you can click the symbol and hear the sound it represents articulated. Careful, though, because they talk all Canadian and stuff. (I kid, I kid!)

EMAIL TO A FRIEND X
Your Email has been sent.
You must add at least one tag to this diary before publishing it.

Add keywords that describe this diary. Separate multiple keywords with commas.
Tagging tips - Search For Tags - Browse For Tags

?

More Tagging tips:

A tag is a way to search for this diary. If someone is searching for "Barack Obama," is this a diary they'd be trying to find?

Use a person's full name, without any title. Senator Obama may become President Obama, and Michelle Obama might run for office.

If your diary covers an election or elected official, use election tags, which are generally the state abbreviation followed by the office. CA-01 is the first district House seat. CA-Sen covers both senate races. NY-GOV covers the New York governor's race.

Tags do not compound: that is, "education reform" is a completely different tag from "education". A tag like "reform" alone is probably not meaningful.

Consider if one or more of these tags fits your diary: Civil Rights, Community, Congress, Culture, Economy, Education, Elections, Energy, Environment, Health Care, International, Labor, Law, Media, Meta, National Security, Science, Transportation, or White House. If your diary is specific to a state, consider adding the state (California, Texas, etc). Keep in mind, though, that there are many wonderful and important diaries that don't fit in any of these tags. Don't worry if yours doesn't.

You can add a private note to this diary when hotlisting it:
Are you sure you want to remove this diary from your hotlist?
Are you sure you want to remove your recommendation? You can only recommend a diary once, so you will not be able to re-recommend it afterwards.
Rescue this diary, and add a note:
Are you sure you want to remove this diary from Rescue?
Choose where to republish this diary. The diary will be added to the queue for that group. Publish it from the queue to make it appear.

You must be a member of a group to use this feature.

Add a quick update to your diary without changing the diary itself:
Are you sure you want to remove this diary?
(The diary will be removed from the site and returned to your drafts for further editing.)
(The diary will be removed.)
Are you sure you want to save these changes to the published diary?

Comment Preferences

  •  I have a PhD in Linguistics and teach accent (6+ / 0-)

    reduction for a living, and I had a hard time making heads or tails of this sentence:

    Another way to say this is to say that for [Ι] and [i], each one is a member of a class of speech sounds that a native speaker will identify as the same sound but that they are not members of the same class.
    I think my problem was that at this point you had introduced phonemic distinctions but hadn't introduced the idea of allophones -- unless I missed it; I was skimming since I know the concepts -- and so I couldn't see how anyone would understand in what sense [i] is a member of a class of speech sounds. There are allophonic variations of [i] but they're kind of tricky to nail down, or so I think.

    When I was teaching Linguistics 101, I usually used the 5 common allophones of [t] to illustrate the idea that we can be making completely different sounds and yet they all 'count' as 'the same sound' -- to a native speaker. You may be doing something like that too; I just didn't see it.

    Please visit: http://www.jkmediasource.org

    by Noisy Democrat on Tue Apr 30, 2013 at 11:18:40 AM PDT

    •  "Accent reduction"? Isn't that the same as learn- (4+ / 0-)
      Recommended by:
      Noisy Democrat, alevei, whaddaya, PeterHug

      ing a different accent?  Does Laurie speak English "without an acccent" on "House"?  How can anybody speak "with no accent"  without speaking an ersatz, artificial accent that no one actually speaks in everyday life?  

      "If you don't read the newspapers, you're uninformed. If you do read the newspapers, you're misinformed." -- M. Twain

      by Oliver St John Gogarty on Tue Apr 30, 2013 at 12:08:27 PM PDT

      [ Parent ]

      •  Actually, I prefer the term "accent acquisition" (4+ / 0-)

        for the reason you pointed out -- what I'm really helping people to do is learn an American accent, not 'reduce' anything, just as when I use my methods on myself to improve my Russian pronunciation, I'm trying to acquire a Russian accent. But 'accent reduction' is such a common term, I often use it just for ease of communication.

        Please visit: http://www.jkmediasource.org

        by Noisy Democrat on Tue Apr 30, 2013 at 12:20:05 PM PDT

        [ Parent ]

    •  Allophonic variations are important. (2+ / 0-)
      Recommended by:
      alevei, whaddaya

      They are the key to understanding that Phoneme is a handy concept but that Phonemes do not actually exist. The concept is abstract, which is one reason that it is so difficult to grasp, and every phoneme is realized as an allophone of that phoneme. Native speakers recognize it no matter how it is realized in speech.

      BTW, I is an allophone of the E that appears in New Zealand English as I. My keyboard doesn't have a symbol for that phoneme, but it is the difference between Sit and Set in most dialects of English. In New Zealand (and some older regional variations of English) speakers say I instead of E, and it is a regular phonemic pattern. I believe I have heard it in some northern rural dialects of England as well.

      W. H. Auden: "We must love one another or die."

      by martyc35 on Tue Apr 30, 2013 at 12:43:15 PM PDT

      [ Parent ]

    •  allophones are members of the same class (2+ / 0-)
      Recommended by:
      Noisy Democrat, PeterHug

      You write:

      "There are allophonic variations of [i] but they're kind of tricky to nail down"
      Indeed they are. Because the variants for [i] articulation are all members of the same class of speech sounds for English speakers, meaning that they are not phonemically distinct from one another, speakers can ignore the differences between them because those differences do not affect meaning. Part of what it means to be proficient is understanding which distinctions you have to pay attention to and which ones to ignore.

      And yes, those slight differences that speakers can ignore with no risk of misunderstanding are allophones, but I don't see why you need that term to understand the concept. The [t] examples you cite are how I learned about allophones in the first place, back when I was an undergrad, but since native speakers of English spend their whole lives ignoring the distinctions between them (e.g., the difference between, say, an aspirated [t] and an unaspirated [t] is of no consequence to us native speakers), I don't find them particularly helpful for students who are grappling with the basic concept of the phoneme. The foreign-language examples I refer to in the diary seem to work a lot better for the students to illustrate the "pay attention to this" and "ignore that" characteristics that help to define the concept of the phoneme, while also illustrating that our distinctions between sounds and how we categorize what constitutes the "same" sound vs. "different" sounds is not at all objective, as evidenced by the variation in what is considered the "same" or "different" across languages.

      Later in the semester, when we talk about things like phonemic splits, i.e. when allophones diverge to the point where the pronunciation differences end up resulting in a difference in meaning depending on which sound is articulated (as in the historical phonemic splits of Germanic k and g, or what may be an eventual split of [æ] articulations in American English, given their historical instability and high salience of pronunciation differences even to native speakers), we can look at specific examples of members of the same class (as [i] and [Ι] are in Spanish) compared to members of different classes (as [i] and [Ι] are in English) and talk about what that means.

      "I couldn't see how anyone would understand in what sense [i] is a member of a class of speech sounds."
      And yet somehow they do. :-)
  •  cool post! (6+ / 0-)

    Spent some time in China where accent is everything. The cosmopolitans from different big cities would occasionally converse in "chinglish" because their Mandarin was divided by accent.

    Excellent post, thanks.

    If you didn't like the news today, go out and make some of your own.

    by jgnyc on Tue Apr 30, 2013 at 11:51:45 AM PDT

  •  speaking of minimal pairs... (6+ / 0-)

    In my language, the following words are all in a minimal-pair relationship with each other:

    bog, cog, fog, hog, jog, log, nog, sog, tog
    The word dog is not.

    I have no idea what their phonemic symbols are, but all of the paired words have a verb sound something like "ah", whereas dog's verb is more like "aw". When I made this observation on a words-oriented email list some time ago, I was shocked to discover that some (many?) of my fellow native speakers made no such distinction.

    Then again, I'm from the part of the nation where Mary, marry, and merry are all pronounced differently from each other. (Mary rhymes with "fairy", marry with "carry", and merry with "ferry".)

    Labor is prior to, and independent of, capital. Capital is only the fruit of labor, and could never have existed if labor had not first existed. Labor is the superior of capital, and deserves much the higher consideration. -- K.Marx A.Lincoln

    by N in Seattle on Tue Apr 30, 2013 at 12:23:01 PM PDT

  •  ooh that is cool. (1+ / 0-)
    Recommended by:
    alevei

    /word nerd

    And we sail and we sail and we never see land, just the rum in the bottle and a pipe in my hand...

    by Mortifyd on Tue Apr 30, 2013 at 05:21:22 PM PDT

  •  What's wrong with folks from UVic... (1+ / 0-)
    Recommended by:
    alevei

    sounding Canadian, eh? Note to non-Canadians: "eh" should be pronounced more like "ey," or "hey" without the "h." Not drawn out either, like Fonzie, but kept rather short when spoken. Americans never seem to get that right.

    Disclosure: I grew up in the very northwestern county of Whatcom in Washington state, right next to the border. In the past, before I lived elsewhere in the US (as I do now), many folks thought I could be Canadian from my accent. Which in the Puget Sound region was like accusing someone of being hosers, like the McKenzie brothers: fightin werds!

    A nice diary, and the first was good too!

    •  If it makes you feel any better (1+ / 0-)
      Recommended by:
      itzadryheat

      my students here in Michigan are pretty nonplussed when I tell them that to people where I come from (Florida), they actually sound like what we would consider Canadian, or at least "indeterminate Northern." Well, maybe it won't make anyone feel better. But a lot of people in Michigan seem to think that the way they talk is "right," so I can't resist pointing out how funny they actually sound to people who aren't from around here.

      And I love the "eh" pronunciation lesson, especially the invocation of the Fonz.

      Thanks for commenting!

      •  What's odd is... (1+ / 0-)
        Recommended by:
        alevei

        that the BC accent has changed in my lifetime. As a teenager in the 70's, I noticed that most folks over the line sounded more or less like most Washingtonians: little apparent accent, no odd inflections (yes, I was and am a nerd). Now, lo these 40 years later, the influx of both foreign and domestic immigrants have shifted the spoken dialect more towards that of eastern Canada, with "ey" being said far more often than formerly, and a more Torontonian sound being heard. Think CBC announcers, and you have it.

  •  I have heard it said that some languages are sung (1+ / 0-)
    Recommended by:
    alevei

    Some languages are tonal like Chinese. When singing in Chinese Mandarin treats tones differently while Cantonese doesn't. Afro-Asiatic and Amerind languages in particular are also interesting in this regard.

    English, depending on which English we are talking about, might be among the atonal languages which are sung for emphasis as for example spoken with a questioning tone.

    In addition to the sounds we make there is the emphasis we put on the sounds, the tone of voice and the shape or contour of the pitch,  the sequence we put the tones in, whether or not they get reduplicated, the volume at which we are speaking, the rapidity with which we are speaking, and whether the accent of the dialect we are using includes things like a drawl or the proper pronunciation of a word borrowed from another language.

    In language shifts vowel contraction and disappearing consonants can result in very different pronunciations of phonemes over time.

    In studying the genealogy of my surname I have come across at least a dozen different spellings as it moved into and out of Old English and Scottish. The Whi it begins with was early on pronounced as Qhy, Quh, Whea and we.

    Live Free or Die --- Investigate, Incarcerate

    by rktect on Thu May 02, 2013 at 04:37:31 AM PDT

Subscribe or Donate to support Daily Kos.

Click here for the mobile view of the site